Tuesday, 26 November 2013

Ramachandran’s nine laws of aesthetics

In his book The Tell-Tale Brain: A Neuroscientist's Quest for What Makes Us Human the neuroscientist V.S. Ramachandran looks at aesthetics from a scientist's viewpoint. He names what he calls his nine laws of aesthetics. Some of them are intuitively known by artists or form part of the art training that artists receive. Others are interesting to think about.




Ramachandran does not hesitate to speculate about the evolution of the laws of aesthetics, and he says so. After all, this is how science progress. Someone throws an idea in the arena and others work on it, either to confirm it or rebuke it.

1. Grouping

This is the fundamental idea that “a visual system tends to group similar elements or features in the image into clusters.”

Ramachandran explains that the principle of grouping evolved to defeat camouflage. The example he gives is the lion hidden behind foliage. There is a clear survival advantage if our brain can put together the disparate yellow splotches of the lion’s body behind the green splotches of the leaves.

Under the principle of grouping, another idea is that” graphic elements suggesting a continued visual contour will tend to be grouped together”.

2. Peak shift

The Law of Peak Shift My relates to how your brain responds to exaggerated stimuli.

This principle explains why we like caricatures. But Ramachandran also invite us to consider that principle make us appreciate works by Van Gogh, Rodin, Gustav Klimt, Henry Moore, or Picasso.

On the neuroscience side, this is probably achieved “by the deliberate exaggeration of posture that may activate—indeed hyper activate—mirror neurons in the superior temporal sulcus.”

Follow some fascinating explanations of experiments on animal behaviour carried out on seagulls by the Nobel Prize–winning biologist Nikolaas Tinbergen. The gull chick pecks on the red spot on its mother’s beak to get food by. Tinbergen used a rectangle with a red spot as a substitute. Tinbergen found that the chick became hyperactive when it was presented with a very long thick stick with three red stripes on the end.

3. Contrast

This one is quite obvious because without contrast, there is no form.

4. Isolation

The “law of isolation” Is when “the artist emphasizes a single source of information—such as colour, form, or motion—and deliberately plays down or deletes other sources.”

Ramachandran believe that sketches are very effective because cells in your primary visual cortex, where the earliest stage of visual processing occurs, only care about lines. Sketches also pass the “attentional bottleneck in your brain”.

5. Peekaboo, or perceptual problem solving

This principle is based on the fact that “you can sometimes make something more attractive by making it less visible.”

This one is easy to understand intuitively. We all experienced the satisfaction of finding out a visual illusion in one of Dali’s paintings for instance.

Ramachandran explains that it starts with the way we see: “When you look at a simple visual scene, your brain is constantly resolving ambiguities, testing hypotheses, searching for patterns, and comparing current information with memories and expectations.”

6. Abhorrence of coincidences

This one is interesting. An illustration of this is that we don’t like a painting when a tree is exactly in the middle. What are the chances of that happening?

7. Orderliness

Here, Ramachandran put together under this heading our abhorrence for deviation from expectations. For the artist it is a matter of balance between too much order (boring) and total chaos (not pleasing).

8. Symmetry

Ramachandran suggests that one explanation comes from…parasites: “Parasitic infestation can profoundly reduce the fertility and fecundity of a potential mate, so evolution places a very high premium on being able to detect whether your mate is infected. If the infestation occurred in early fetal life or infancy, one of the most obvious externally visible signs is a subtle loss of symmetry. Therefore, symmetry is a marker, or flag, for good health, which in turn is an indicator of desirability.”

The author then tackles the question apparent paradox that a lack of symmetry may also be appealing at times. His answer is that “the symmetry rule applies only to objects, not to large-scale scenes.”

9. Metaphors

Metaphors used extensively in visual art. Ramachandran expresses is fascination for the fact that a “visual metaphor is probably understood by the right hemisphere long before the more literal-minded left hemisphere can spell out the reasons.”

Even if the author does not make the express link, I think that the Peekaboo, or perceptual problem solving principle plays a role in our appreciation of metaphor. A metaphor is a kind of puzzle and our brain must take pleasure in resolving these riddles.

In conclusion, Ramachandran explains that these principles in a single work can enhance each other. This is what he calls “resonance”.

Even if the part on art is only one chapter, I would recommend that you read this book. It is one of the most interesting reads around and Ramachandran has a gift to explain in simple words some very complex pieces of research. This is the work of a true humanist, looking at so many aspects of our world through his neuroscientist experience.

Related resources

Get the book from Amazon in the US (Associate program link)

4 comments:

  1. total of 'eight laws of aesthetic experience' (analogous to the Buddha's ... Correspondence: V.S. Ramachandran, Center For Brain and Cognition,

    ReplyDelete
  2. Can you send me please anything where you found this - in his paper on this he mentions preference for a generic viewpoint , says noting about the tree being in the middle -- and if he does say it somewhere I would like to know as only people with amnesia would draw it this way -- thanx ania lian
    Abhorrence of coincidences
    This one is interesting. An illustration of this is that we don’t like a painting when a tree is exactly in the middle. What are the chances of that happening

    ReplyDelete
  3. Ania, the author starts the part on "Abhorrence of Coincidence" with an anecdote from his youth, when he produced a palm tree growing between two hill and Mrs. Vanit, his art teacher, told him this did not looked right. The book (I read it on the Kindle) has 2 drawings. The author's explanation is that "you could only see the scene [...]" which is symmetrical "from one vantage point, whereas you could see the "as-symmetrical figure "from any number of vantage points. One viewpoint is unique and one is generic." And he adds that "your brain always tries to find a plausible alternate, generic interpretation to avoid the coincidence. In this case it doesn't find one and so the image isn't pleasing". (Chapter 8 of the book).

    ReplyDelete

I would like to know what you think about my art and my posts. Feel free to ask questions. No advertising links in comments please.